nurture$54085$ - translation to γερμανικά
Diclib.com
Λεξικό ChatGPT
Εισάγετε μια λέξη ή φράση σε οποιαδήποτε γλώσσα 👆
Γλώσσα:

Μετάφραση και ανάλυση λέξεων από την τεχνητή νοημοσύνη ChatGPT

Σε αυτήν τη σελίδα μπορείτε να λάβετε μια λεπτομερή ανάλυση μιας λέξης ή μιας φράσης, η οποία δημιουργήθηκε χρησιμοποιώντας το ChatGPT, την καλύτερη τεχνολογία τεχνητής νοημοσύνης μέχρι σήμερα:

  • πώς χρησιμοποιείται η λέξη
  • συχνότητα χρήσης
  • χρησιμοποιείται πιο συχνά στον προφορικό ή γραπτό λόγο
  • επιλογές μετάφρασης λέξεων
  • παραδείγματα χρήσης (πολλές φράσεις με μετάφραση)
  • ετυμολογία

nurture$54085$ - translation to γερμανικά

RELATIVE IMPORTANCE OF AN INDIVIDUAL'S INNATE QUALITIES ("NATURE" IN THE SENSE OF NATIVISM OR INNATISM) AS COMPARED TO AN INDIVIDUAL'S PERSONAL EXPERIENCES ("NURTURE" IN THE SENSE OF EMPIRICISM OR BEHAVIORISM)
Nature versus nurture debate; Nature vs nurture; Nurture; Nature vs. nurture; Nature vrs. nurture; Nurture or nature; Nature-nurture controversy; Nature-nurture; Nurture versus nature; Nature/nurture debate; Nature-nurture debate; Nature nurture debate; Nature or nurture; Nature versus Nurture; Nature vs. Nurture; Nurture v nature; Nature v nurture; Heredity and environment; Nature-nurture problem; Nature–nurture problem; Nature-versus-nurture; Nature and nurture; Blank-slatism; Nature–nurture debate; Nature-nurture dichotomy; Nurture vs nature; Nature Vs. Nurture
  • More realistic "homogenous mudpie" view of heritability.
  • The "two buckets" view of heritability.
  • This chart illustrates three patterns one might see when studying the influence of genes and environment on traits in individuals. Trait A shows a high sibling correlation, but little heritability (i.e. high shared environmental variance ''c''<sup>2</sup>; low heritability ''h''<sup>2</sup>). Trait B shows a high heritability since the correlation of trait rises sharply with the degree of genetic similarity. Trait C shows low heritability, but also low correlations generally; this means Trait C has a high nonshared environmental variance ''e''<sup>2</sup>. In other words, the degree to which individuals display Trait C has little to do with either genes or broadly predictable environmental factors—roughly, the outcome approaches random for an individual. Notice also that even identical twins raised in a common family rarely show 100% trait correlation.
  • alt=

nurture      
n. Nahrung; Pflege; Erziehung

Ορισμός

nurture
I. n.
1.
Nourishment, food, diet.
2.
Training, education, instruction, discipline, tuition schooling, breeding.
3.
Nursing, nourishing, tender care, attention.
II. v. a.
1.
Feed, nourish, nurse, tend.
2.
Train, educate, instruct, school, rear, breed, discipline, bring up, train up.

Βικιπαίδεια

Nature versus nurture

Nature versus nurture is a long-standing debate in biology and society about the balance between two competing factors which determine fate: genetics (nature) and environment (nurture). The alliterative expression "nature and nurture" in English has been in use since at least the Elizabethan period and goes back to medieval French.

The complementary combination of the two concepts is an ancient concept (Ancient Greek: ἁπό φύσεως καὶ εὐτροφίας). Nature is what people think of as pre-wiring and is influenced by genetic inheritance and other biological factors. Nurture is generally taken as the influence of external factors after conception e.g. the product of exposure, experience and learning on an individual.

The phrase in its modern sense was popularized by the Victorian polymath Francis Galton, the modern founder of eugenics and behavioral genetics when he was discussing the influence of heredity and environment on social advancement. Galton was influenced by On the Origin of Species written by his half-cousin, the evolutionary biologist Charles Darwin.

The view that humans acquire all or almost all their behavioral traits from "nurture" was termed tabula rasa ('blank tablet, slate') by John Locke in 1690. A blank slate view (sometimes termed blank-slatism) in human developmental psychology, which assumes that human behavioral traits develop almost exclusively from environmental influences, was widely held during much of the 20th century. The debate between "blank-slate" denial of the influence of heritability, and the view admitting both environmental and heritable traits, has often been cast in terms of nature versus nurture. These two conflicting approaches to human development were at the core of an ideological dispute over research agendas throughout the second half of the 20th century. As both "nature" and "nurture" factors were found to contribute substantially, often in an inextricable manner, such views were seen as naive or outdated by most scholars of human development by the 21st century.

The strong dichotomy of nature versus nurture has thus been claimed to have limited relevance in some fields of research. Close feedback loops have been found in which nature and nurture influence one another constantly, as seen in self-domestication. In ecology and behavioral genetics, researchers think nurture has an essential influence on nature. Similarly in other fields, the dividing line between an inherited and an acquired trait becomes unclear, as in epigenetics or fetal development.